Abstract
Reentrant or feedback pathways between cortical areas carry rich and varied information about behavioral context, including attention, expectation, perceptual task, working memory and motor commands. Neurons receiving such inputs effectively function as adaptive processors that are able to assume different functional states according to the task being executed. Recent data suggest that the selection of particular inputs, representing different components of an association field, enable neurons to take on different functional roles. In this review we discuss the various top-down influences exerted on the visual cortical pathways and highlight the dynamic nature of the receptive field, which allows neurons to carry information that is relevant to the current perceptual demands.
The functional properties of cortical neurons are not fixed. Rather, they can be thought of as adaptive processors, changing their function according to the behavioral context, and their responses reflect the demands of the perceptual task being performed. Cortical neurons are subject to top-down influences of attention, expectation and perceptual task. “Top-down” refers to cognitive influences and higher order representations that impinge upon earlier steps in information processing. Such influences represent a reversal of the central dogma of sensory information processing, which is based on feedforward connections along a hierarchy of cortical areas representing progressively more complex aspects of the visual scene. But superimposed on the feedforward pathways there are reentrant or feedback pathways that convey higher order information to antecedent cortical areas. The top-down signal carries a rich amount of information that facilitates the interpretation of the visual scene and that enables the visual system to build a stable representation of the objects within it, despite rapid and continuous eye movements. It facilitates our ability to segment the complex arrangement of multiple objects and backgrounds in the visual scene. In addition, the top-down signal plays a role in the encoding and recall of learned information. The resulting feedforward signals carried by neurons convey different meanings about the same visual scene according to the behavioral context. This idea is in stark contrast with the classical notion of a hierarchy of visual cortical areas -- where information is conveyed in a feedforward fashion to progressively higher levels in the hierarchy, beginning with the analysis of simple attributes such as contrast and orientation, and leading to more complex functional properties from one stage to the next -- and implies that vision is an active process. As we analyze visual scenes we set up countercurrent streams of processing, with the resulting percept reflecting the set of functional states of all the areas in the visual cortical hierarchy. In this review we consider the receptive field properties that are subject to top-down influences, the nature of the information that is conveyed by reentrant pathways, and how the information carried by neurons depends on behavioral context. Over longer time periods receptive fields can change to accommodate alterations in visual experience. These lines of evidence point towards an evolving view of the nature of the receptive field, which includes contextual influences and emphasizes its dynamic nature, with neurons taking on different properties in response to experience and expectation.
Top-down influences are conveyed across a series of descending pathways covering the entire neocortex and are relayed through thalamic nuclei (Figure 1). The feedforward connections define a hierarchy of visual cortical areas, beginning with primary visual cortex (V1) and ascending through two primary pathways, a ventral pathway, which is involved with object recognition, and a dorsal pathway, which is involved with visually guided movements and attentional control. For every feedforward connection there is a reciprocal feedback connection that carries information about the behavioral context.
Figure 1. Feedback pathways carrying top-down information.
Processing visual information involves feed forward connections across a hierarchy of cortical areas (represented by the blue arrows) beginning in primary visual cortex (V1), which in turn receives input from the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN). The feed forward connections extend through a ventral pathway into the temporal lobe and a dorsal pathway into the parietal and prefrontal cortex. Matching these feedforward connections are a series of reciprocal feedback connections (represented by the red arrows), providing descending top-down influences that mediate “reentrant” processing. Feedback is seen in direct cortico-cortical connections (those directed towards V1), in projections from V1 to the LGN, and in interactions between cortical areas mediated by the pulvinar. Information about motor commands, or efference copy, is fed to the sensory apparatus by a pathway involving the superior colliculus (SC), medial dorsal nucleus of the thalamus (MD) and frontal eye fields (FEF). In addition to direct reciprocal connections, for example from V2 to V1, feedback can cascade over a succession of areas, for example PF to FEF to V4 to V2 to V1. As outlined in this review, a diversity of information is conveyed across these pathways, including attention, expectation, perceptual task and efference copy. (Adapted from Gilbert, Figure 25-7B in Principles of Neuroscience, Kandel, Schwartz, Jessell, Siegelbaum and Hudspeth).
The Receptive Field
Top-down influences take into account the nature of stimulus dependent properties in any sensory cortical area. There is an emerging view that in the early stages of visual cortical processing, rather than doing a local analysis of simple features, neurons can integrate information over large parts of the visual field, and that neurons in these areas can show selectivity for complex stimulus configurations. The integrative properties of cortical neurons are reflected in their selectivity for stimulus context. Contextual influences refer to the ways by which the perceptual qualities of a local feature are affected by surrounding scene elements and the way in which global scene characteristics affect the responses of neurons to local features. They play important roles in perceptual grouping, perceptual constancies, contour integration, surface segmentation and shape recognition. The most profound effects of top-down control are exerted on contextual influences. This has led to a change in our thinking about the role and prevalence of top-down influences across the visual cortical hierarchy, from initial studies suggesting that they are negligible at early stages of cortical processing to current studies showing substantial changes in neural responses with shifts in attention, with expectation and perceptual task.
Understanding how such cognitive influences affect neuronal function requires understanding the character of the receptive field. The visual receptive field is the part of the retina where a stimulus can cause the neuron to respond with a train of action potentials. The characterization of the receptive field is dependent on the nature of the stimulus used to measure it. A simple stimulus, such as an oriented line segment, will activate a neuron over a small part of the visual field (known as the “minimum response field”, which for superficial layer V1 parafoveal receptive fields is in the order of 0.5° in diameter), but similar stimuli outside this area, which by themselves will not activate the neuron, can greatly affect the neuron’s response when presented jointly with a stimulus in the centre of the receptive field. These modulatory influences can be either facilitatory or inhibitory, and the direction and size of the effect depends on the position of the flanking stimulus relative to the receptive field core1. As a consequence, neurons’ responses are as dependent on the characteristics of global contours and surfaces as they are on the attributes of local features within the minimum response field, and these contextual influences can extend over relatively large regions of the visual field. Contextual influences in areas V1 and V2 have been implicated in intermediate level vision, including contour integration (the assembly of contour elements into global shapes) and surface segmentation (the separation between object surfaces and their backgrounds)2-8. The extent of these contextual influences varies according to the level of stimulus complexity and attentional state9. One has to keep contextual influences in mind when considering the role of top-down influences in altering a neuron’s response properties.
The function of a neuron is also characterized by its tuning to a range of stimuli, such as different line orientations, directions of movement or colors. One can extend this to any stimulus space and determine the responsiveness of a neuron to stimuli in different points within that space. This has been applied to determine a neuron’s selectivity for the shapes of complex objects or for the configuration of complex stimuli consisting of multiple line segments. Beyond examining the shape of a neuron’s tuning, one can use other measures to characterize a neuron’s stimulus selectivity and to relate that selectivity to perception10. One is mutual information – the degree to which a neuron’s response predicts stimulus identity, quantified in bits. Another is ideal observer analysis, which allows one to relate a neuron’s discriminability in a stimulus space, its “neurometric” curve, or to the animal’s discrimination performance, its “psychometric” curve. Top-down influences also affect these measures of neuronal function, and as a result, change the nature of the information that neurons convey.
The cortical source and circuitry underlying contextual influences have been vigorously debated5, 11-17. We have proposed that long range intrinsic cortical connections provide a substrate for interactions across the visual field, and have a spatial extent and columnar specificity that is consistent with the contextual influences and with the Gestalt rules of perceptual grouping5, 11, 12, 14. Some have argued that these influences originate from higher order cortical areas, based on their timing relative to stimulus onset. It is not clear that timing is a reliable indicator of the source of a signal given the fast conduction velocities of feedback projections. An alternative explanation is that a signal delay is due to the time required for the network to shift from one stable state to another, with foreground and background interactions requiring time to evolve18. Delayed influences have been seen with stimuli involving texture segmentation and contour saliency5, 19, but for stimulus configurations without complex backgrounds contextual effects have been observed from the onset of responses20 (see Figure 2).
Figure 2. Task-dependent changes in neural tuning and information content in V1.
Monkeys were trained to perform two different tasks with a visual stimulus consisting of 5 lines – a central line flanked by two collinear and two parallel lines. Each of the pairs of flanking lines were presented in one of 5 offsets relative to the central line fixed in the receptive field of a recorded neuron, forming a total of 25 stimulus conditions. From these stimuli the animals were cued to perform either a 3-line bisection task, based on the relative positions of the 3 parallel lines, or a vernier discrimination task, based on the relative positions of the 3 collinear lines. The bisection task involves judging to which of the two flanking parallel lines the central line is closer, and the vernier task involves judging the direction of offset of the central line relative to the two collinear lines. (a) The tuning of neurons to the offset of the side-flanks was measured when the animal performed either the 3-line bisection task, where the side flank position was relevant to the task (solid red line), or the vernier discrimination task, where the side flank position was irrelevant to the task (dashed black line). The cell shown in this example was more modulated in its response to side flank offset position when the animal performed the 3-line bisection task (difference in response shown in blue). (b) The change in tuning of a V1 cell to the end-flank offset position when the animal performed the vernier discrimination task, where the tuning was relevant to the task (solid red line) versus when it performed the 3-line bisection task, where the tuning was task irrelevant (dashed black line). (c) The difference in tuning for task relevant and task irrelevant conditions was characterized in terms of mutual information, where the population of recorded neurons carried more information relative to side flank tuning (blue ×) or vernier tuning (red +) in the task relevant condition than in the task irrelevant condition. A series of Monte-Carlo simulations where the responses were randomly assigned to the two different tasks are shown in the blue and red clouds, which are located on the diagonal and far from the experimental conditions. (d) The difference in response between the task relevant and task irrelevant conditions arose from the outset of the neurons’ responses, indicating that the cortical state for performing a given task was set in advance of stimulus onset. (from Li et al, 200420 fig 2, 3, 4 and 7).
The nature of top-down influences and their effect on receptive field properties
Top-down influences include different forms, such as attention, expectation and perceptual task. They are seen at all stages in the visual hierarchy, including primary visual cortex, and reaching as far back as the lateral geniculate nucleus21, 22. The effect of these influences is to alter receptive field properties and the information carried by neural ensembles. As a consequence, vision can be thought of as an active process, requiring expectation or hypothesis testing in order to interpret the visual scene. Some contextual influences have been proposed to arise from a predictive coding strategy, where higher levels in the cortical hierarchy make predictions about lower-level activity, and some neurons carry an error signal between the prediction and the stimulus generated activity23, 24. Top-down influences assume a number of forms, and there is a rich amount of information conveyed from higher order to lower order areas:
Spatial attention
Top-down control is traditionally associated with spatial attention. Its effect has largely been characterized in terms of gain control— enhancement of neural responses – as well as suppression of responses outside of the focus of attention25, 26. The utility of spatial attention is to allow us to select behaviorally relevant stimuli and to analyze specific parts of the visual field27. The consequent enhancement of neural responses is seen in a number of cortical areas, including V1, V2, V4, MT and IT5, 26, 28-40 and it provides a mechanism for selection of behaviorally relevant stimuli from competing distracters41. While earlier studies have suggested that higher order visual areas in the cortical hierarchy are more subject to attentional influences than earlier stages42, the magnitude of attentional effects is highly dependent on the nature of the task and the configuration of the stimulus20, 26, 35, 38, 43-46. Attentional effects are more profound when there is competition between multiple stimuli26, 45. In V1, this is when contextual influences are involved5, 26, 35, 38, 47. One should therefore consider the effects of attention on lateral interactions, instead of their influence on feedforward properties such as the orientation of a line segment. For example, two collinear lines, one inside and one outside the receptive field, will produce a stronger response relative to that elicited by a single line centered within the receptive field. This facilitation depends on whether the lines are at an attended location and on the discrimination task performed at that location, resulting in several-fold differences between responses obtained with “attend to” and “attend away” conditions35. Attentional influences become more evident with increasing stimulus complexity26 and depend on the precise geometric relationships between stimulus components5.
Object oriented and feature oriented attention
Rather than acting as a ‘searchlight’, attention can highlight discriminability of features belonging to the same object (object oriented attention), or components sharing similar properties (feature oriented attention), such as color, orientation or direction of movement. Feature based attention highlights the components of a scene sharing the same attribute, and distributes cognitive resources broadly across the visual scene, rather than the restricted spotlight of spatial attention31, 48-50. The effect may be specific to cortical areas that deal with the attended feature, such as color in area V4, direction of movement in area MT50-53 or with the attended object, such as the fusiform face area or parahippocampal place area54. Object oriented attention increases the perceptual saliency of the components of an entire object, rather than the features incorporated within a fixed spotlight. Attending to an object encompasses all the features belonging to the object3, 54-61, and as measured with fMRI, the cortical effects of attention to a feature can spread throughout the visual field, even to regions lacking a visual stimulus62, 63.
The Gestalt psychology movement recognized the importance of the whole influencing the perceptual quality of the parts, essentially reversing the direction of information flow whereby the representation of the object precedes the representation of its components64. Object expectation may play an important role in the segmentation of the visual scene. Because of the complexity of the visual environment, the most difficult task of object recognition is not the identification of an object but the association of the contour elements and surfaces belonging to the object and separating these from the object’s background. Thus, while object recognition itself can, in theory, be accomplished by feedforward mechanisms alone65, top-down processes, or a “countercurrent stream” of information flow, is required for proper scene segmentation66 where objects have to be identified in complex scenes consisting of many objects. Models that incorporate recurrent processing can help to resolve an extremely challenging task for the visual system: grouping and segmenting elements within the visual scene.
Perceptual task
Even when attending to the same location and receiving an identical stimulus, the tuning of neurons can change according to the perceptual task that is being performed. This form of top-down control allows the network to engage stimulus components that are relevant to the task, and to discard influences from components that are irrelevant to the task. The task dependent change in tuning of neurons can be analyzed in terms of a change in task relevant information in neuronal signalling. This implies that the functional roles of neurons are not fixed, but instead that they are adaptive processors, running different programmes in differing behavioral contexts. By changing the perceptual task based on the same visual stimulus, one sees responses that are influenced by different stimulus components20. As shown in Figure 2, when presenting a central target line flanked by 2 parallel lines and 2 collinear lines, animals can perform either a 3-line bisection task based on the parallel lines or a vernier discrimination task based on the collinear lines. Neurons change their tuning according to the task being performed, showing more modulation to changes in position of the task relevant components (the parallel lines when performing the 3-line bisection task, or the collinear lines when performing the vernier discrimination task) than to changes in position of the task irrelevant stimulus components.
Another example of this task dependency is in a curve tracing task, in which responses are enhanced for neurons with receptive fields lying along the attended than along the unattended contour3. A task involving detection of a contour in a complex background enhances the contour related facilitation in responses of V1 neurons. The perceptual saliency, or detectability, of such a contour increases with the number of collinear line segments, and this correlates with the increase in neuronal responses as the contour is lengthened. The facilitatory influence of the collinear line segments is much larger when the animal performs a contour detection task than when it carries out an unrelated task5. Although we emphasize the specificity of the task in generating the enhanced neural responses, one might think that these observations fall under the rubric of object oriented attention. Regardless of whether one calls this object-oriented attention or a task-dependent top-down influence, it is important to emphasize that the effect is to cause neurons to change their tuning to the characteristics of the stimuli within the area of visual space that is attended.
Recent electrophysiological studies have suggested that the frontal eye field is the cortical locus for attentional selection of a target among distracters67, 68. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field has shown that the back propagation of the induced signals from the prefrontal cortex to visual areas is dependent on the task being performed on a given stimulus, reflecting task-specific modulatory effects of expectation69. Anatomical studies have also shown segregated pathways projecting from frontal cortex to area V4 and MT, which may carry different top-down signals for processing different stimulus features70.
The idea that neurons multiplex their function in a task-dependent fashion, that is, at different times they select one out of a battery of functional properties, may be general to all areas of the cerebral cortex (for the auditory cortex71). Recordings in prefrontal cortex have demonstrated that neurons can be tuned to multiple categorical distinctions, so that the same neuron can exhibit different categorical representations as the task changes72 (Figure 3). Establishing the generality of this phenomenon to other areas depends on using an experimental design in which neurons’ selectivities are measured under different behavioral contexts. This is described in the following section, where neurons’ shape selectivity is determined while animals are searching for different shapes.
Figure 3.
Neurons in the prefrontal cortex carry out different functions in accordance with task. Top, monkeys were trained to discriminate between “dog” and “cat” categories in a delayed match to sample task as images were morphed from dog to cat prototypes, or between “sports car” and “sedan” categories as imaged were morphed from sports car to sedan prototypes. Bottom, an individual neuron in the prefrontal cortex showed similar responses to images on one side of the category boundary and distinct responses to images on opposite sides of the category boundary. The differential responses during the delay period between dog/cat categories or sports car/sedan categories support the idea of neuronal multitasking. (from Cromer et al, 201072 figure 2).
Object expectation
When animals are cued to look for a specific shape, the shape selectivity of neurons in V1 changes to a form that approximates the cued shape or a portion of that shape. Evidence in support of object expectation in producing selectivity for specific geometric forms comes from an experiment in which animals were trained to identify a cued contour embedded in a complex environment. The cue consisted either of a straight line, a circle or a wave shape. The shape selectivity of V1 neurons was measured before the correct and false targets were presented in complex backgrounds in either hemifield, at which time the animal made a saccade towards the correct target. The important finding of this experiment was first, that neurons in V1 showed selectivity for complex shape geometries (not just single oriented line segments), and second, that this selectivity could be altered, for individual neurons and for the population of superficial layer neurons as a whole, by changing shape expectation8. This process suggests that expectation of an object creates a set of filters that are selective for the object’s components and thus, a role of top-down processes in object recognition73. The idea is further supported by the transfer of perceptual learning between objects with shared components74.
These experiments demonstrate that even at the earliest stages in visual cortical processing neurons are selective for more complex geometries than single oriented line segments, and that their shape selectivity depends on object expectation. In effect, neurons become selective for components of expected objects, and object recognition involves a countercurrent stream of processing, with top down anticipatory influences dynamically creating the appropriate set of lower level filters, and the feedforward connections from these filters collectively creating the representation of the full object. This emerging view contrasts with the dogma of hierarchical, bottom-up visual processing.
Efference copy
We see the world as stable, even as our eyes scan the visual scene, causing movement of scene features across our retinas. This is because a copy of the motor instruction to execute an eye movement, known as the efference copy or corollary discharge, is sent to the sensory apparatus to “subtract” the movement signal, thereby cancelling out any sensation of object movement due to eye movement. In the last few years the efference copy pathway, involving the superior colliculus, the medial dorsal nucleus of the thalamus and frontal eye fields, has been elucidated75, 76. The effect of this signal is to shift receptive field position (for neurons in the parietal cortex) in the direction of the eye movement77. An alternative mechanism for perceptual stability is one involving a predictive mapping of attention to selected targets78, although the shift in the locus activation of neurons is nonetheless powerful evidence of top-down influence on receptive field properties based on motor plan. Shifting cortical receptive fields in anticipation of eye movements has been seen in areas of parietal cortex and frontal eye fields79-84. Thus, for some neurons, even the property of receptive field location is not fixed, and shifting receptive fields plays a valuable role in perceptual stability.
Working memory, associative memory and perceptual learning
The way a cortical area responds to a stimulus depends on prior experience and current task. An excellent example of this is one in which animals were trained to associate a pattern of moving dots with a stationary arrow. Ordinarily neurons in area MT respond to stimuli moving in a particular direction and are not responsive to stationary stimuli. But in animals trained in this associative task, MT neurons respond well to the stationary stimulus, indicating that their activity reflects not just the external stimulus but also cognitive state, visual imagery and stimulus associations85 (Figure 4). Another example is where neurons in frontal eye fields retain “memory responses” in the absence of a visual stimulus but represent locations of intended saccades, that is, they respond to stimuli located in positions where the receptive field will be at the end of the saccade86, or the target of attentional selection87. Longer term influences of learning, in particular perceptual learning, have been shown to alter response properties as early as V1 (for review see Gilbert and Li, 201213). Although perceptual learning is outside the scope of this review, top-down influences play an important role in its mechanism. They are required for the encoding of the learned information as well as in its recall, as the neuronal properties associated with learning are only present when the animal is performing the trained task6, 20, 38.
Figure 4. Learned association generates recall-related activity in area MT.
Area MT normally responds to moving stimuli. However, when trained to associate a moving stimulus, a set of dots moving in a particular direction, with a static stimulus, an arrow (top), neurons become activated by the static stimulus. Bottom A, A neuron in area MT responds to and shows directional tuning to both the moving dot stimulus (red) and the static arrow stimulus (blue). B, for this neuron, polar plot showing tuning to direction of movement (red) and to arrow orientation (blue). (from Schlack and Albright, 200785 figure 2).
Dynamic encoding of information at the network level
A useful way to think about the effect of top-down influences is in terms of the information they convey and impart upon their target neurons. Information theory provides a measure of the extent an ideal observer can categorize a stimulus based on the spike count from a recorded neuron during one trial. Top-down influences affect neuronal tuning in a way that enables neurons to carry more information about the stimulus being discriminated. Neurons can increase the degree of modulation of their responses over a set of stimuli, making these responses more informative about stimulus identity.
The idea that a neuron is an adaptive processor, changing the calculation it performs in accordance to the top-down instruction received from higher order cortical areas, has attendant with it that the neuron’s line label is not fixed. The line label idea suggests that when a neuron fires it is signaling the presence of a stimulus possessing the neuron’s preferred attribute (orientation preference, for example), and the strength of its firing indicates the closeness of the stimulus to that attribute. But if the top-down signal causes neurons to change the meaning of the information they carry, then these neurons are effectively changing their line label. How can this not distort the analysis of the visual image if neurons are constantly changing their function? The answer lies in the fact that the higher order areas sent the instruction for these neurons to perform a particular calculation, so the return signal is “interpreted” by these areas as the result of that calculation, and is not confused with other operations those neurons perform.
Beyond the effect of top-down influences on the functional properties of individual neurons, neuronal ensembles can be induced to carry more information by altering their correlation structure, that is, the spatial and temporal distribution of correlated activity over the network of neurons within and across cortical areas. Neurons are variable in their responses to a given stimulus, and as more neurons participate in encoding the stimulus, the variability can be averaged out to provide better signal to noise ratios. But this depends on the ability of neurons to be independent from one another. The optimal information content would require zero or low noise correlations. There is, however, a significant amount of noise correlation88-91, so a decrease in noise correlations induced by top-down influences would increase the amount of information encoded by the neuronal ensemble92-95. Decorrelation in the trial to trial variability of responses can allow groups of neurons to average out this variability and improve the signal to noise ratio. This benefit depends on whether neurons are similarly tuned as noise correlation between differently tuned neurons can increase coding efficiency92, 93, 96.
Attention and perceptual learning have been shown to reduce noise correlations, although this has been an area of some debate97-100. Even more task-specific effects are seen on noise correlations between cortical sites that are relevant to the task being performed, and these changes are larger than those associated with merely attending to the stimulus101. In area MT, noise correlations between a pair of neurons receiving identical visual stimuli can either increase or decrease depending on which of two orthogonal axes the monkey is cued to perform a motion detection task102.
Top-down influences go well beyond specifying the locus of spatial attention and changing neuronal firing rates. The recurrent pathways that convey these influences must be capable of conveying much more information than a locus to be attended. By the same token, top-down influences cause neurons at the antecedent stages in the cortical hierarchy to alter the nature of the information in their signals. This is not simply a matter of gain control, but involves alterations in tuning that enable neurons to carry more information about stimulus components that are relevant to the task at hand, to take on selectivity for features that are components of expected objects, and to maintain a stable representation of the world in the face of continual eye movements. The increase in task relevant information is contributed in part by the changes in tuning of individual neurons and in part by the change in the structure of correlations across the neuronal ensemble.
Different forms of top-down influences have been documented in different cortical areas, and these effects are relevant to the functional properties of these areas. But all cortical areas, and even the thalamus, can exhibit profound top-down influences. Based on early findings on the lack of attentional effects in V1, along with findings of strong effects in V4 and MT, it has been suggested that attentional influences get progressively stronger along the visual pathway42. However, more recent findings, based on more complex stimuli and behavioral paradigms, have called this idea into question, and have suggested that all areas in the hierarchy are equally subject to top-down influences. It is becoming increasingly evident that attention effects are seen early in the visual pathway21, 22, 26, 35, 43, 44, 46, 103. The way in which these influences are manifest depends on the functional role of each cortical area: contour integration in V1, responses to movement direction in MT, modulation by eye position in parietal areas, and so on.
Circuit mechanisms of top-down control
Many studies on top-down influences have focused on the enhancement or change in gain of responses induced by attention, which is equivalent to the stimulus being increased in contrast50, 52, 104, 105. The influence of attention on stimuli within the receptive field has been described in the ‘biased competition’ model41. In this model objects in the visual field compete for computing resources, and an object can “win” on the strength of its saliency (“bottom up” attention or pop-out) or behavioral relevance (top-down control). Related to the idea of biased competition is a normalization model of attention, which involves a multiplicative scaling of responses to multiple stimuli in the receptive field, and attention affects the strength of the normalization106, 107. These models assume that attention does not affect the stimulus selectivity of neurons. But top-down influences can alter the information carried in neuronal signals, which is distinct from a gain control. For example, changes in a neuron’s tuning to the specific components of the stimulus that are relevant to the task being performed have been observed20, 38, rather than a generalized increase in response to attended stimuli. Attention can change stimulus selectivity in addition to changing gain of responses108. It is therefore useful to have a model that can account for the specificity of top-down influences for different contextual components and for a neuron’s ability to select a subset among all its inputs in order to exhibit different functional properties. According to this model, although a neuron receives thousands of inputs from intrinsic connections, only a fraction of these are expressed under a particular behavioral context. Interactions between reentrant connections from higher order cortical areas and intrinsic circuits enable the network to gate the connections that are appropriate for the task at hand, with different functional networks operating under different task conditions. As a consequence neurons multiplex their function in a state-dependent manner, and constitute adaptive processors running different operations under the instruction of feedback from higher order cortical areas109.
The contextual influences that mediate higher order, complex receptive field properties in V1 involve lateral interactions across a topographically organized region, and they have the consequence of perceptual grouping, such as that involved with linking line segments to global contours. The interactions follow precise geometric rules, showing facilitatory influences for neurons with receptive fields lying along collinear or cocircular contours. This mode of interaction is known as the “association field”110. This is a general entity that has been identified in V1, but that is likely to have an analog in all cortical areas. The idea underlying the association field is a linkage between elements that are systematically represented, topographically, over each cortical area. Lateral interactions between these elements allow perceptual linkage or association of pieces of information. The lateral interactions may be mediated by a plexus of long-range horizontal connections within V1. These connections are formed by pyramidal neurons, whose axons extend for long distances parallel to the cortical surface, and link neurons with widely separated receptive fields11, 14, 111-114. Because of their extent and columnar specificity (they connect neurons of similar orientation preference11, 14, 115, 116), they are ideal conveyors of the contextual influences that enable contour integration12. Although the horizontal connections provide an anatomical framework for a range of contextual interactions, the observation that these interactions are subject to top-down control suggests that feedback signals can alter the effective connectivity of horizontal connections.
We have proposed that reentrant inputs dynamically modify intrinsic cortical connections, allowing the appropriate associations to be made under different behavioral contexts. A possible reason for the existence of horizontal connections is that they allow such changes in connectivity within the network, as opposed to each cell having a large receptive field generated by a fixed set of feedforward connections. This idea has been implemented in models of cortical circuitry, in which changes in the gain of horizontal connections by feedback allows subsets of neuronal inputs to be selectively expressed18. It also accounts for the time course of contextual interactions, where delayed components of neuronal responses are due to the time required for the network to move from one stable state to another, rather than a function of the conduction time required to get information from a distant, more central source. Finally, it provides a mechanism for contour integration and saliency18. The interaction between feedback and horizontal connections also suggests a mechanism for perceptual learning. During the encoding of learned information the recurrent input acquires the appropriate mapping to intrinsic connections, and during the recall of the learned information this relationship allows the appropriate inputs to be gated and the target neuron to assume the appropriate functional properties. In V1 the association field mediates contour integration and saliency, and the top-down input allows for sub-components of the association field to be gated, leading to the manifestation of different shape selectivities. In other areas the association field would be defined by the properties and the kind of information that are topographically mapped in that area, and by the relationship between the long range horizontal connections and that map.
Many of the task and expectation dependent effects described above can be explained by an input selection mechanism. By selecting components of the association field, neurons can express contextual influences that are relevant to the task being performed. A contour detection task enhances collinear interactions and suppresses influences from non collinear elements in the background5. A shape discrimination task induces neurons to select collinear influences when the cue is a line and cocircular influences when the cue is a circle8. By selection of components of the association field over multiple nodes in the horizontal network, neurons in V1 can take on selectivity for complex shapes, including wave like shapes with reversals in curvature. The selective influence of parallel lines in a 3-line bisection task and collinear lines in a vernier discrimination task20 can be mediated by changing the effective connectivity of task relevant inputs. This idea is supported by an experiment involving recording from an array of electrodes, where the interactions between cortical sites are measured by cross-correlation analysis (based on the relative timing of spikes between pairs of neurons) or coherence between local field potentials (LFPs) measured at different sites. Changing the perceptual task with the identical visual stimulus strongly influences correlation strength. Perceptual grouping tasks enhance LFP coherence between parallel sites in 3-line bisection, and between collinear sites in contour detection. Perceptual segregation decreases LFP coherence between collinear sites as seen in a vernier discrimination task101 (Figure 5). This is similar to the expectation-dependent changes seen in noise correlations102. Although some measures of coherence suggest that attention decreases cortical interactions117, the effect of top-down influences depends on the nature of the task and the way in which different cortical sites are engaged in the task. Further support of this idea comes from fMRI measures of coupling between distant cortical sites representing separated stimuli in a task requiring integration of the two stimuli118.
Figure 5.
Task dependent changes in local field potential coherence and noise correlations in area V1. Neurons were recorded with a 96 electrode array in animals trained on the 3-line bisection or vernier discrimination tasks based on the 5 line stimulus (a, b) or on the contour detection task based on a series of collinear line segments embedded in a background of randomly positioned and oriented lines (c). The effective connectivity between cortical sites representing parallel flanks (a) and collinear flanks (b) was measured by calculating the coherence between local field potentials (LFPs) at different frequencies. The graphs in the center column represent LFP-LFP coherence during the response interval from 100 to 500 ms in the task relevant (red) and task irrelevant (black) conditions. Operations involving grouping of parallel sites, 3-line bisection, or of collinear sites, contour detection, give stronger coherence in the task relevant condition. Operations involving segregation of collinear sites, vernier discrimination, produces weaker coherence in the task relevant condition. The difference in coherence in the 3-line bisection and vernier tasks was seen not only during the entire response period but in the interval preceding stimulus presentation, indicating top-down setting of lateral cortical interactions in advance of the appearance of the stimulus. (d), Noise correlations show task dependent differences. Calculated as Fisher information as a function of changes in stimulus bar position for the three task conditions (black, attend-away, green, attention to the receptive field location, red, performing the relevant task at the receptive field location), the V1 network carried more information about the stimulus when the animal performed the task, and roughly equal contributions to the increase in information came from the changes in neuronal tuning (dotted red line) and from the changes in noise correlation (solid red line). (from Ramalingam et al, 2013101).
Changes in effective connectivity mediated by top-down influences relates to the idea that neural synchrony is the neural code for perceptual grouping and segmentation119-125, although some studies have failed to confirm this idea102, 126-129. It has been proposed that perceptual grouping is achieved by synchronizing the activity of neurons representing the grouped features130, 131, and that neuronal synchrony plays important roles in sensorimotor integration132-134. Synchrony in itself may be more a reflection of the dynamic connectivity leading to task dependent alterations in neural tuning rather than the information being carried by the relative timing of action potentials per se. The two may in fact be related, with alterations in effective connectivity underlying the task-dependent changes in tuning. Selective attention can also provide a solution to the “superposition problem”, where contour components belonging to one object have to be associated with one another and perceptually separated from components that belong to the object’s background. The role of attention in synchronization is seen in animals performing a color change detection task, in which there is gamma band synchronization between cortical sites encoding the behaviorally relevant stimulus135. Also, top-down influences can affect effective connectivity between cortical areas. Just as attention can increase gamma band synchronization within V4, it increases synchronization between the frontal eye fields and V4136, 137. This idea is supported in human subjects by fMRI based correlations of BOLD background connectivity between cortical areas, which is specific to task and cortical area138. It is important to emphasize that top-down influences don’t just alter effective connectivity in general, they can selectively and differentially change the effective connectivity between cortical sites that are task relevant101.
Signals that represent top-down influences are observed following the cue directing the task or expectation and before the stimulus presentation99, 101, 139, 140. The pre-stimulus task-dependent activity suggests that subthreshold signals set the cortical “state” for executing the calculation that is appropriate for the behavioral context, and that a given percept results from the set of states assumed by the entire network of cortical areas. This view contrasts with the traditional idea that perception results from the activity of a small number of cells at the top of the visual hierarchy. Instead, the percept arises from the global set of cortical states and task-specific interactions between multiple cortical areas.
The connectivity mediating top-down instructions is likely to include cortico-cortical feedback connections. For area V1, for example, although the strongest feedback arises from area V2, there are a number of cortical areas that provide direct recurrent input to V1, including those in the ventral pathway, such as areas V4 and IT, and areas in the dorsal pathway, including area MT14, 141-143. The feedback projection from area MT to V1 has been implicated in visual awareness144. The ventral pathway inputs could provide information about object expectation and the dorsal pathway inputs could provide information about attentional locus or saliency maps. In addition, other areas, such as prefrontal cortex, could provide executive control over a perceptual task, and the sites of transmission from prefrontal cortex to posterior areas depend on the nature of the task69. Though prefrontal cortex is not directly connected to V1, it could exert its influence by a cascade of connections descending through the parietal lobe. Other non-cortical sources of recurrent input have been suggested, such as the pulvinar145, 146. Multiple sources are likely to be involved in top-down control, but as indicated above, they must be capable of carrying the richness of information involved in not only spatial attention but expectation and perceptual task. (Figure 1)
Summary
The existence of such a varied array of top-down influences and their profound effect on the functional properties of neurons (as well as on their interactions within neuronal ensembles) raises a host of questions for further investigation: What are the sources of the various types of top-down control and what are the pathways by which this control is exerted? What is the nature of the signal that is conveyed along these recurrent pathways? What are the synaptic and network mechanisms by which feedforward, recurrent and intrinsic cortical connections interact to enable adaptive changes in neuronal function? The challenge is to address these questions in the context of the intact, functioning system and to do so in behaving animals.
By selecting different sets of inputs neurons take on different functions. The source of top-down influences can be widespread, either by direct connections from different cortical areas, or by a cascade of inputs originating from many more areas. In effect a large part of the cerebral cortex can exert influences over individual neurons within a particular area, with multiple descending inputs interacting with intrinsic cortical connections (Figure 6). As such, each neuron is a microcosm of the brain as a whole, with synapses carrying information originating from far flung brain regions. This mode of operation has important implications for our understanding of the cortical mechanisms underlying all sensory modalities and behaviors, and its dysfunction may be the cause of behavioral disorders.
Figure 6.
Top-down influences dynamically change effective connectivity within and between cortical areas, allowing neurons to select inputs, and take on functional properties, that are appropriate for the immediate behavioral context. As a result each cortical area and each neuron within that area is an adaptive processor, continuously changing its line label to serve different functions. Right, Long range horizontal connections link distant points in each cortical map, mediating an association field that provides a set of potential interactions. The association field in V1 is represented by the gray cocircular and collinear lines and by the fields of oriented line segments on either side of the central black neuron. The underlying circuit is represented by the long range horizontal connections formed by excitatory neurons (triangles) and disynaptic connections involving inhibitory neurons (circles). Depending on the top-down instruction, different sets of inputs can be gated according to the state of feedback (represented by the green connections coming from higher order cortical areas), so that under different tasks the black neuron may select either the red or blue inputs. Because of the multiple sources of long range inputs coming from within the same cortical area and from many other cortical areas, and because these influences can cascade over multiple nodes, each neuron effectively becomes a microcosm of nearly the entire brain. Left, multiple layers of such interactions operate across the entire visual pathway, each cortical area containing its own gate-able association field, and top-down interactions cascade across the layers (feedforward pathways are represented by the blue connections between cortical “planes” and feedback pathways are represented by the red connections), not just between nearby cortical areas but also by longer range connections that skip over multiple stages (not shown). Each cortical area is represented here as a 2-dimensional network, but because of their laminar structure different layers tend to be responsible for feedforward connections (superficial cortical layers) and feedback connections (deep cortical layers).
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by US National Institutes of Health grant EY007968 (C.D.G.), a grant from the James S. McDonnell Foundation (C.D.G.), the National Natural Science Foundation of China grant 31125014 (W.L.) and the 111 Project B07008 (W.L.).
Glossary
- Visual cortical hierarchy
Refers to the hierarchy of cortical areas in the classical model of the cortical representation of visual information beginning with the primary visual cortex and ascending through two pathways, a ventral pathway extending into the temporal lobe, which is involved with object recognition, and a dorsal pathway extending into the parietal lobe, which is involved with visually directed movement and spatial attention.
- Reentrant or feedback pathways
Refers to the processing strategy whereby the product of an ongoing computation at one cortical level is analyzed by the next level. The resultant information is then sent back to the initial level to influence its further computation. This is also sometimes referred to as countercurrent processing streams.
- Intermediate level vision
Visual processing has been characterized as involving three stages, low level vision, the analysis of simple attributes such as contrast, orientation, movement and color, intermediate level vision, which involves contour integration and surface segmentation, and high level vision, which involves object recognition.
- Distracters
In a complex visual scene, some objects are attended (the targets) and others (the distracters) are unattended, but the distracters can compete with the target for attentional resources.
- Hemifield
Visual cortical areas are topographically mapped, particularly those at earlier stages in the cortical hierarchy. A cortical area in one hemisphere receives input from the contralateral half of the visual field, or hemifield.
- Line label
The property or information represented by a neuron. Different neurons represent different values, and the strength of their firing indicates how close the stimulus is to that value.
- Noise
The variability in neurons’ responses to a given stimulus. If different neurons with similar functional properties have independent noise, an ensemble of such neurons can carry more information about a stimulus than a single neuron.
- Local field potential
The electrical field generated by a population of neurons, with signals having components spanning a spectrum of frequencies. Local field potentials originate from the integrated currents coming from synaptic activation and from action potentials in dendrites, cell somata and axons.
Biographies
Charles Gilbert is the Arthur and Janet Ross professor at The Rockefeller University. He is a member of the National Academy of Sciences and the American Academy of Arts and Sciences. His work focuses on the brain mechanisms of visual perception and learning at the molecular, circuit and perceptual levels. He studies the way in which the brain analyzes visual images, how this analysis is shaped by higher order cognitive influences and by perceptual learning, and the circuit mechanisms of experience dependent plasticity of the adult visual cortex.
Wu Li is the Director of the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning at Beijing Normal University, China. He received his bachelor's degree in biophysics from University of Science and Technology of China, and his Ph.D. in neurobiology from Shanghai Institute of Physiology, Chinese Academy of Sciences. He trained as a postdoctoral researcher with Gerald Westheimer at University of California, Berkeley, USA; with Christian Wehrhahan and Peter Thier at Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Germany; with Charles Gilbert at Rockefeller University, USA. He studies the neural mechanisms underlying visual perception using psychophysical and electrophysiological approaches.
Footnotes
Online 'at-a-glance' summary
Bulleted summary
- Rather than having a fixed functional role, neurons are adaptive processors, changing their function according to behavioral context.
- Vision is an active process, where higher order cognitive influences affect the operations performed by cortical neurons.
- Visual pathways operate bidirectionally, with each feedforward connection matched by a feedback or reentrant connections going from higher to lower order cortical areas.
- Top-down influences include various forms of attention, including spatial, object oriented and feature oriented attention.
- Top-down influences are not limited to attention but mediate a much broader range of functional roles, including perceptual task, object expectation, scene segmentation, efference copy, working memory, and the encoding and recall of learned information.
- The effect of top-down influences is to change the information conveyed by neurons, both by alteration of the tuning of their responses to stimulus attributes and by changing the structure of correlations over neuronal ensembles.
- All areas of the visual pathway, except for the retina, are subject to top-down influences, including early cortical stages of visual processing such as the primary visual cortex and the lateral geniculate nucleus, and all areas along the dorsal and ventral visual cortical pathways. Each area contains an association field of potential interactions, and expresses a subset of these interactions to execute different functions.
- The sources of top-down influences are widespread, with each area providing information reflecting the functional properties of that area. As a consequence, even a single neuron can be viewed as a microcosm of activity occurring throughout the visual pathway..
- We propose that the circuit mechanism of top-down control and adaptive processing involves a gating of intrinsic cortical circuits within an area mediated by long range feedback connections to that area. By selecting a subset of inputs, a neuron can express different components of its association field, and as a result take on different functional roles.
References
- 1.Kapadia MK, Westheimer G, Gilbert CD. Spatial distribution of contextual interactions in primary visual cortex and in visual perception. J Neurophysiol. 2000;84:2048–2062. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.4.2048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Zipser K, Lamme VA, Schiller PH. Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1996;16:7376–7389. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-22-07376.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Roelfsema PR, Lamme VA, Spekreijse H. Object-based attention in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Nature. 1998;395:376–381. doi: 10.1038/26475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Zhou H, Friedman HS, von der Heydt R. Coding of border ownership in monkey visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2000;20:6594–6611. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-17-06594.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Li W, Piech V, Gilbert CD. Contour saliency in primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2006;50:951–962. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Li W, Piech V, Gilbert CD. Learning to link visual contours. Neuron. 2008;57:442–451. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Zhang NR, von der Heydt R. Analysis of the context integration mechanisms underlying figure-ground organization in the visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2010;30:6482–6496. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5168-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.McManus JN, Li W, Gilbert CD. Adaptive shape processing in primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:9739–9746. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105855108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Womelsdorf T, Anton-Erxleben K, Treue S. Receptive field shift and shrinkage in macaque middle temporal area through attentional gain modulation. J Neurosci. 2008;28:8934–8944. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4030-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Thomson EE, Kristan WB. Quantifying stimulus discriminability: a comparison of information theory and ideal observer analysis. Neural Comput. 2005;17:741–778. doi: 10.1162/0899766053429435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Columnar specificity of intrinsic horizontal and corticocortical connections in cat visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1989;9:2432–2442. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-07-02432.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Li W, Gilbert CD. Global contour saliency and local colinear interactions. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88:2846–2856. doi: 10.1152/jn.00289.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gilbert CD, Li W. Adult visual cortical plasticity. Neuron. 2012;75:250–264. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Stettler DD, Das A, Bennett J, Gilbert CD. Lateral connectivity and contextual interactions in macaque primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2002;36:739–750. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01029-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hupe JM, et al. Feedback connections act on the early part of the responses in monkey visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2001;85:134–145. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.1.134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bair W, Cavanaugh JR, Movshon JA. Time course and time-distance relationships for surround suppression in macaque V1 neurons. J Neurosci. 2003;23:7690–7701. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-20-07690.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Angelucci A, Bressloff PC. Contribution of feedforward, lateral and feedback connections to the classical receptive field center and extra-classical receptive field surround of primate V1 neurons. Prog Brain Res. 2006;154:93–120. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)54005-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Piech V, Li W, Reeke GN, Gilbert CD. A network model of top-down influences on local gain and contextual interactions in visual cortex. Submitted for Publication. 2012 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1317019110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lamme VA. The neurophysiology of figure-ground segregation in primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1995;15:1605–1615. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-02-01605.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Li W, Piech V, Gilbert CD. Perceptual learning and top-down influences in primary visual cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:651–657. doi: 10.1038/nn1255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.O'Connor DH, Fukui MM, Pinsk MA, Kastner S. Attention modulates responses in the human lateral geniculate nucleus. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:1203–1209. doi: 10.1038/nn957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.McAlonan K, Cavanaugh J, Wurtz RH. Guarding the gateway to cortex with attention in visual thalamus. Nature. 2008;456:391–394. doi: 10.1038/nature07382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Rao RP, Ballard DH. Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:79–87. doi: 10.1038/4580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Spratling MW. Predictive coding as a model of response properties in cortical area V1. J Neurosci. 2010;30:3531–3543. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4911-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Chen Y, et al. Task difficulty modulates the activity of specific neuronal populations in primary visual cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:974–982. doi: 10.1038/nn.2147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Motter BC. Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. J Neurophysiol. 1993;70:909–919. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.70.3.909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Posner MI, Snyder CR, Davidson BJ. Attention and the detection of signals. J Exp Psychol. 1980;109:160–174. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Moran J, Desimone R. Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. Science. 1985;229:782–784. doi: 10.1126/science.4023713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Mountcastle VB, Motter BC, Steinmetz MA, Sestokas AK. Common and differential effects of attentive fixation on the excitability of parietal and prestriate (V4) cortical visual neurons in the macaque monkey. J Neurosci. 1987;7:2239–2255. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-07-02239.1987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Spitzer H, Desimone R, Moran J. Increased attention enhances both behavioral and neuronal performance. Science. 1988;240:338–340. doi: 10.1126/science.3353728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Chelazzi L, Miller EK, Duncan J, Desimone R. A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex. Nature. 1993;363:345–347. doi: 10.1038/363345a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Chelazzi L, Miller EK, Duncan J, Desimone R. Responses of neurons in macaque area V4 during memory-guided visual search. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:761–772. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.8.761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Treue S, Maunsell JH. Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST. Nature. 1996;382:539–541. doi: 10.1038/382539a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Luck SJ, Chelazzi L, Hillyard SA, Desimone R. Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1997;77:24–42. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.1.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ito M, Gilbert CD. Attention modulates contextual influences in the primary visual cortex of alert monkeys. Neuron. 1999;22:593–604. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80713-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.McAdams CJ, Maunsell JH. Effects of attention on orientation-tuning functions of single neurons in macaque cortical area V4. J Neurosci. 1999;19:431–441. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-01-00431.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Reynolds JH, Chelazzi L, Desimone R. Competitive mechanisms subserve attention in macaque areas V2 and V4. J Neurosci. 1999;19:1736–1753. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-05-01736.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Crist RE, Li W, Gilbert CD. Learning to see: experience and attention in primary visual cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:519–525. doi: 10.1038/87470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Treue S. Neural correlates of attention in primate visual cortex. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24:295–300. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01814-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Reynolds JH, Desimone R. Interacting roles of attention and visual salience in V4. Neuron. 2003;37:853–863. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00097-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1995;18:193–222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Maunsell JH, Cook EP. The role of attention in visual processing. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2002;357:1063–1072. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Gandhi SP, Heeger DJ, Boynton GM. Spatial attention affects brain activity in human primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1999;96:3314–3319. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.6.3314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Poghosyan V, Ioannides AA. Attention modulates earliest responses in the primary auditory and visual cortices. Neuron. 2008;58:802–813. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kastner S, De Weerd P, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG. Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. Science. 1998;282:108–111. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5386.108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Watanabe T, et al. Task-dependent influences of attention on the activation of human primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95:11489–11492. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.19.11489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ito M, Westheimer G, Gilbert CD. Attention and perceptual learning modulate contextual influences on visual perception. Neuron. 1998;20:1191–1197. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80499-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Chelazzi L, Duncan J, Miller EK, Desimone R. Responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex during memory-guided visual search. J Neurophysiol. 1998;80:2918–2940. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.6.2918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Motter BC. Neural correlates of attentive selection for color or luminance in extrastriate area V4. J Neurosci. 1994;14:2178–2189. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-04-02178.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Treue S, Martinez Trujillo JC. Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex. Nature. 1999;399:575–579. doi: 10.1038/21176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Bulthoff I, Bulthoff H, Sinha P. Top-down influences on stereoscopic depth-perception. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:254–257. doi: 10.1038/699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Reynolds JH, Pasternak T, Desimone R. Attention increases sensitivity of V4 neurons. Neuron. 2000;26:703–714. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81206-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Giesbrecht B, Woldorff MG, Song AW, Mangun GR. Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention. Neuroimage. 2003;19:496–512. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00162-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.O'Craven KM, Downing PE, Kanwisher N. fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection. Nature. 1999;401:584–587. doi: 10.1038/44134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Duncan J. Selective attention and the organization of visual information. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1984;113:501–517. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.113.4.501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Egly R, Driver J, Rafal RD. Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1994;123:161–177. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.123.2.161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Blaser E, Pylyshyn ZW, Holcombe AO. Tracking an object through feature space. Nature. 2000;408:196–199. doi: 10.1038/35041567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Reynolds JH, Alborzian S, Stoner GR. Exogenously cued attention triggers competitive selection of surfaces. Vision Res. 2003;43:59–66. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00403-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Yantis S, Serences JT. Cortical mechanisms of space-based and object-based attentional control. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13:187–193. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00033-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Poort J, et al. The role of attention in figure-ground segregation in areas V1 and V4 of the visual cortex. Neuron. 2012;75:143–156. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Somers DC, Dale AM, Seiffert AE, Tootell RB. Functional MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1999;96:1663–1668. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.4.1663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Saenz M, Buracas GT, Boynton GM. Global effects of feature-based attention in human visual cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:631–632. doi: 10.1038/nn876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Serences JT, Boynton GM. Feature-Based Attentional Modulations in the Absence of Direct Visual Stimulation. Neuron. 2007;55:301–312. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Wertheimer M. Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt. Psychologische Forschung. 1923;4:301–350. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Riesenhuber M, Poggio T. Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:1019–1025. doi: 10.1038/14819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Borenstein E, Ullman S. Combined top-down/bottom-up segmentation. IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell. 2008;30:2109–2125. doi: 10.1109/TPAMI.2007.70840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Zhou H, Desimone R. Feature-based attention in the frontal eye field and area V4 during visual search. Neuron. 2011;70:1205–1217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.04.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Monosov IE, Trageser JC, Thompson KG. Measurements of simultaneously recorded spiking activity and local field potentials suggest that spatial selection emerges in the frontal eye field. Neuron. 2008;57:614–625. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Morishima Y, et al. Task-specific signal transmission from prefrontal cortex in visual selective attention. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:85–91. doi: 10.1038/nn.2237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Ninomiya T, Sawamura H, Inoue K, Takada M. Segregated pathways carrying frontally derived top-down signals to visual areas MT and V4 in macaques. J Neurosci. 2012;32:6851–6858. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6295-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Fritz J, Elhilali M, Shamma S. Active listening: Task-dependent plasticity of spectrotemporal receptive fields in primary auditory cortex. Hearing Research. 2005;206:159–176. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2005.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Cromer JA, Roy JE, Miller EK. Representation of multiple, independent categories in the primate prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2010;66:796–807. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Ullman S. Object recognition and segmentation by a fragment-based hierarchy. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Golcu D, Gilbert CD. Perceptual learning of object shape. J Neurosci. 2009;29:13621–13629. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2612-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Sommer MA, Wurtz RH. What the brain stem tells the frontal cortex. II. Role of the SC-MD-FEF pathway in corollary discharge. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:1403–1423. doi: 10.1152/jn.00740.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Wurtz RH, Sommer MA. Identifying corollary discharges for movement in the primate brain. Prog Brain Res. 2004;144:47–60. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)14403-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Duhamel JR, Colby CL, Goldberg ME. The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. Science. 1992;255:90–92. doi: 10.1126/science.1553535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Rolfs M, Jonikaitis D, Deubel H, Cavanagh P. Predictive remapping of attention across eye movements. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:252–256. doi: 10.1038/nn.2711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Sommer MA, Wurtz RH. Influence of the thalamus on spatial visual processing in frontal cortex. Nature. 2006;444:374–377. doi: 10.1038/nature05279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Kusunoki M, Goldberg ME. The time course of perisaccadic receptive field shifts in the lateral intraparietal area of the monkey. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89:1519–1527. doi: 10.1152/jn.00519.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Umeno MM, Goldberg ME. Spatial processing in the monkey frontal eye field. I. Predictive visual responses. J Neurophysiol. 1997;78:1373–1383. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.3.1373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Nakamura K, Colby CL. Updating of the visual representation in monkey striate and extrastriate cortex during saccades. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:4026–4031. doi: 10.1073/pnas.052379899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Colby CL, Duhamel JR, Goldberg ME. Visual, presaccadic, and cognitive activation of single neurons in monkey lateral intraparietal area. J Neurophysiol. 1996;76:2841–2852. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.5.2841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Batista AP, Buneo CA, Snyder LH, Andersen RA. Reach plans in eye-centered coordinates. Science. 1999;285:257–260. doi: 10.1126/science.285.5425.257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Schlack A, Albright TD. Remembering visual motion: neural correlates of associative plasticity and motion recall in cortical area MT. Neuron. 2007;53:881–890. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Umeno MM, Goldberg ME. Spatial processing in the monkey frontal eye field. II. Memory responses. J Neurophysiol. 2001;86:2344–2352. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.5.2344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Armstrong KM, Chang MH, Moore T. Selection and maintenance of spatial information by frontal eye field neurons. J Neurosci. 2009;29:15621–15629. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4465-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Zohary E, Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. Correlated neuronal discharge rate and its implications for psychophysical performance. Nature. 1994;370:140–143. doi: 10.1038/370140a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Bair W, Zohary E, Newsome WT. Correlated firing in macaque visual area MT: time scales and relationship to behavior. J Neurosci. 2001;21:1676–1697. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-05-01676.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Gawne TJ, Kjaer TW, Hertz JA, Richmond BJ. Adjacent visual cortical complex cells share about 20% of their stimulus-related information. Cereb Cortex. 1996;6:482–489. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.3.482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Lee D, Port NL, Kruse W, Georgopoulos AP. Variability and correlated noise in the discharge of neurons in motor and parietal areas of the primate cortex. J Neurosci. 1998;18:1161–1170. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-03-01161.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Abbott LF, Dayan P. The effect of correlated variability on the accuracy of a population code. Neural Comput. 1999;11:91–101. doi: 10.1162/089976699300016827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Averbeck BB, Latham PE, Pouget A. Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:358–366. doi: 10.1038/nrn1888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Oram MW, Foldiak P, Perrett DI, Sengpiel F. The 'Ideal Homunculus': decoding neural population signals. Trends Neurosci. 1998;21:259–265. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(97)01216-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Panzeri S, Schultz SR, Treves A, Rolls ET. Correlations and the encoding of information in the nervous system. Proc Biol Sci. 1999;266:1001–1012. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Romo R, Hernandez A, Zainos A, Salinas E. Correlated neuronal discharges that increase coding efficiency during perceptual discrimination. Neuron. 2003;38:649–657. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00287-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Gu Y, et al. Perceptual learning reduces interneuronal correlations in macaque visual cortex. Neuron. 2011;71:750–761. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Cohen MR, Maunsell JH. Attention improves performance primarily by reducing interneuronal correlations. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:1594–1600. doi: 10.1038/nn.2439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Mitchell JF, Sundberg KA, Reynolds JH. Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in macaque area V4. Neuron. 2009;63:879–888. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Poort J, Roelfsema PR. Noise correlations have little influence on the coding of selective attention in area V1. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:543–553. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Ramalingam N, McManus JNJ, Li W, Gilbert CD. Top-down modulation of lateral interactions in visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2013;33:1773–1789. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3825-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Cohen MR, Newsome WT. Context-dependent changes in functional circuitry in visual area MT. Neuron. 2008;60:162–173. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Jack AI, Shulman GL, Snyder AZ, McAvoy M, Corbetta M. Separate modulations of human v1 associated with spatial attention and task structure. Neuron. 2006;51:135–147. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Herrmann K, Montaser-Kouhsari L, Carrasco M, Heeger DJ. When size matters: attention affects performance by contrast or response gain. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:1554–1559. doi: 10.1038/nn.2669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Williford T, Maunsell JH. Effects of spatial attention on contrast response functions in macaque area V4. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:40–54. doi: 10.1152/jn.01207.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Lee J, Maunsell JH. A normalization model of attentional modulation of single unit responses. PLoS One. 2009;4:e4651. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Reynolds JH, Heeger DJ. The normalization model of attention. Neuron. 2009;61:168–185. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Murray SO, Wojciulik E. Attention increases neural selectivity in the human lateral occipital complex. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:70–74. doi: 10.1038/nn1161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Gilbert CD, Sigman M. Brain states: top-down influences in sensory processing. Neuron. 2007;54:677–696. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Field DJ, Hayes A, Hess RF. Contour integration by the human visual system: evidence for a local "association field". Vision Res. 1993;33:173–193. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90156-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Morphology and intracortical projections of functionally characterised neurones in the cat visual cortex. Nature. 1979;280:120–125. doi: 10.1038/280120a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Clustered intrinsic connections in cat visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1983;3:1116–1133. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-05-01116.1983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Rockland KS, Lund JS. Widespread periodic intrinsic connections in the tree shrew visual cortex. Science. 1982;215:1532–1534. doi: 10.1126/science.7063863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Rockland KS, Lund JS. Intrinsic laminar lattice connections in primate visual cortex. J Comp Neurol. 1983;216:303–318. doi: 10.1002/cne.902160307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Ts'o DY, Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Relationships between horizontal interactions and functional architecture in cat striate cortex as revealed by cross-correlation analysis. J Neurosci. 1986;6:1160–1170. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.06-04-01160.1986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Bosking WH, Zhang Y, Schofield B, Fitzpatrick D. Orientation selectivity and the arrangement of horizontal connections in tree shrew striate cortex. J Neurosci. 1997;17:2112–2127. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-06-02112.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Chalk M, et al. Attention reduces stimulus-driven gamma frequency oscillations and spike field coherence in V1. Neuron. 2010;66:114–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Haynes JD, Tregellas J, Rees G. Attentional integration between anatomically distinct stimulus representations in early visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:14925–14930. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0501684102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Castelo-Branco M, Goebel R, Neuenschwander S, Singer W. Neural synchrony correlates with surface segregation rules. Nature. 2000;405:685–689. doi: 10.1038/35015079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Gail A, Brinksmeyer HJ, Eckhorn R. Contour decouples gamma activity across texture representation in monkey striate cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:840–850. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.9.840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Kreiter AK, Singer W. Stimulus-dependent synchronization of neuronal responses in the visual cortex of the awake macaque monkey. J Neurosci. 1996;16:2381–2396. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-07-02381.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Singer W, Gray CM. Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1995;18:555–586. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.003011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.von der Malsburg C, Schneider W. A neural cocktail-party processor. Biol Cybern. 1986;54:29–40. doi: 10.1007/BF00337113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Eckhorn R, et al. Coherent oscillations: a mechanism of feature linking in the visual cortex? Multiple electrode and correlation analyses in the cat. Biol Cybern. 1988;60:121–130. doi: 10.1007/BF00202899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Gray CM, Konig P, Engel AK, Singer W. Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties. Nature. 1989;338:334–337. doi: 10.1038/338334a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Roelfsema PR, Lamme VA, Spekreijse H. Synchrony and covariation of firing rates in the primary visual cortex during contour grouping. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:982–991. doi: 10.1038/nn1304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Palanca BJ, DeAngelis GC. Does neuronal synchrony underlie visual feature grouping? Neuron. 2005;46:333–346. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Dong Y, Mihalas S, Qiu F, von der Heydt R, Niebur E. Synchrony and the binding problem in macaque visual cortex. J Vis. 2008;8(30):31–16. doi: 10.1167/8.7.30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Lamme VA, Spekreijse H. Neuronal synchrony does not represent texture segregation. Nature. 1998;396:362–366. doi: 10.1038/24608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie AE, Desimone R. Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science. 2001;291:1560–1563. doi: 10.1126/science.1055465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Steinmetz PN, et al. Attention modulates synchronized neuronal firing in primate somatosensory cortex. Nature. 2000;404:187–190. doi: 10.1038/35004588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Bland BH, Oddie SD. Theta band oscillation and synchrony in the hippocampal formation and associated structures: the case for its role in sensorimotor integration. Behavioural brain research. 2001;127:119–136. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00358-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Riehle A, Grun S, Diesmann M, Aertsen A. Spike synchronization and rate modulation differentially involved in motor cortical function. Science. 1997;278:1950–1953. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5345.1950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Roelfsema PR, Engel AK, Konig P, Singer W. Visuomotor integration is associated with zero time-lag synchronization among cortical areas. Nature. 1997;385:157–161. doi: 10.1038/385157a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Womelsdorf T, Fries P, Mitra PP, Desimone R. Gamma-band synchronization in visual cortex predicts speed of change detection. Nature. 2006;439:733–736. doi: 10.1038/nature04258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, Desimone R. High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science. 2009;324:1207–1210. doi: 10.1126/science.1171402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Fries P, Womelsdorf T, Oostenveld R, Desimone R. The effects of visual stimulation and selective visual attention on rhythmic neuronal synchronization in macaque area V4. J Neurosci. 2008;28:4823–4835. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4499-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Al-Aidroos N, Said CP, Turk-Browne NB. Top-down attention switches coupling between low-level and high-level areas of human visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:14675–14680. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1202095109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Ress D, Backus BT, Heeger DJ. Activity in primary visual cortex predicts performance in a visual detection task. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:940–945. doi: 10.1038/78856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Kastner S, Pinsk MA, De Weerd P, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG. Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron. 1999;22:751–761. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80734-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Rockland KS, Saleem KS, Tanaka K. Divergent feedback connections from areas V4 and TEO in the macaque. Vis Neurosci. 1994;11:579–600. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800002480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Rockland KS, Knutson T. Feedback connections from area MT of the squirrel monkey to areas V1 and V2. J Comp Neurol. 2000;425:345–368. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Rockland KS, Van Hoesen GW. Direct temporal-occipital feedback connections to striate cortex (V1) in the macaque monkey. Cereb Cortex. 1994;4:300–313. doi: 10.1093/cercor/4.3.300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Pascual-Leone A, Walsh V. Fast backprojections from the motion to the primary visual area necessary for visual awareness. Science. 2001;292:510–512. doi: 10.1126/science.1057099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Saalmann YB, Pinsk MA, Wang L, Li X, Kastner S. The pulvinar regulates information transmission between cortical areas based on attention demands. Science. 2012;337:753–756. doi: 10.1126/science.1223082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Sherman SM, Guillery RW. Distinct functions for direct and transthalamic corticocortical connections. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:1068–1077. doi: 10.1152/jn.00429.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]